A recent meta-analysis by three economists, commonly, if misleadingly, referred to as the Johns Hopkins study, suggested that lockdowns had little to no effect on Covid-19 mortality. Critics accused the authors of ideological bias in interpreting, or even manipulating the evidence to fit their pre-existing beliefs. One might think that by this point, figuring out the effectiveness of lockdowns would be a simple matter of looking at the data, the facts. But while the question might seem like a purely empirical one, it’s not, argue Lucie White and Philippe van Basshuysen.
A recent meta-analysis by three economists suggesting that lockdowns have had “little to no effect on COVID-19 mortality” has generated an intense and polarised reaction across the internet and media outlets. Various potential flaws with the study have been, by now, well documented. But there have also been suggestions that the authors did not only make mistakes in their study, but had deliberately manipulated the evidence to fit with their own biased anti-lockdown preconceptions. On the other side, commentators made accusations that the study had been unduly ignored because it contradicted pre-existing narratives about the efficacy of lockdowns.
___
___
SUGGESTED READING
Were the Covid measures worth it?
By Stephen John
Strong disagreement about the efficacy of lockdowns is nothing new. The mathematical models that were used to justify initial lockdown decisions have been pilloried as deeply flawed and as vastly overestimating the amount of COVID deaths, while others have maintained that lockdowns were a necessary means of saving millions of lives. But we might think that this disagreement could be definitively resolved once we had access to data – and could actually see, after the fact, how well lockdowns had worked. It’s concerning, then, that the availability of empirical work seems to have done very little to resolve the disagreement. The polarised response continues, and is simply shifted to accusations of bias in how the data is interpreted.
What are we to make of this continuing disagreement concerning the facts of the matter? We might be tempted to explain it away by pointing to key features surrounding the current disagreement. Much has been made of the fact that the study has not yet been peer reviewed, and it is certainly the case that the need for rapid information during a fast moving pandemic has led to an unprecedented reliance on preprints. Or we might point to the way that information about the pandemic and government response is disseminated, and discussed, on social media – a highly polarising environment which naturally yields polarised responses to studies like the most recent one. But looking at other cases, more removed from our current concerns, where there are also stark disagreements about empirical evidence, could provide some insight into how we should understand the current arguments about the efficacy of lockdowns.
One such example is the contentious debate in economics about a minimum wage. Proponents held that introducing (or raising) a minimum wage would diminish poverty by improving the circumstances of the worst off. Opponents believed that a minimum wage would drive down demand for low-skilled workers, increasing unemployment and disadvantaging the worst off. But the economist Milton Friedman was optimistic that this disagreement could be resolved. After all, he said, we all have the same goal in mind – we want a living wage for all. The question is just about whether minimum wage legislation is a good means of achieving this goal – and to find this out, we just need to see what the effects actually are when a minimum wage is introduced.
Join the conversation